Em Proceedings of the Computer Support for Collaborative Learning (CSCL) 1999 Conference , C. Hoadley & J. Roschelle (Eds.) Dezembro 12-15, Universidade De Stanford, Palo Alto, Califórnia. Mahwah, Nj: Associados De Lawrence Erlbaum.

Projeto Centrado Atividade: Para uma estrutura teórica para CSCL

Bernard R. Gifford e Noel D. Enyedy

Universidade de Califórnia em Berkeley

Sumário: Os computadores não tiveram ainda o impacto profundo na prática da sala de aula que foi predita. Dado o potencial provado da instrução computer-mediated, que pode esclarecer a falta de progresso? Este papel explora os underpinnings teóricos de muitos dos ambientes computer-mediated existentes da aprendizagem e sugere que as teorias da aprendizagem que se encontram atrás deles conduzem aos projetos que não cabem com nem mudam as estruturas básicas da participação da sala de aula. Nós discutimos preferivelmente para o projeto centrado atividade (ACD), um modelo do projeto para ambientes collaborative suportados computador da aprendizagem, baseado nas seguintes suposições: essa atividade é mediada por ferramentas cultural, essa atividade deve conceptualized em um número de níveis interdependentes, e essa compreensão conceptual é estabelecida primeiramente social. Nós então crítica nosso próprio ambiente existente da aprendizagem, o ambiente do inquérito da probabilidade, do perspective do ACD.
Keywords: teoria da atividade, estrutura do projeto, análise do discurso

Introdução

Na última década, o campo da instrução invested uma quantidade enorme de energia e de recursos ao projeto de ambientes computer-mediated da aprendizagem. Os findings deste corpo do trabalho são consistentes e incentivando. De acordo com artigos e muitas meta-análises de ambos os investigadores individuais a respeito da eficácia pedagogical de materiais instrutivos por computador, os ambientes computer-mediated da aprendizagem aumentaram o estudante que aprende e interesse (por exemplo Berson, 1996; Kulik, 1994; Weller, 1996). Entretanto, apesar do rhetoric optimistic que os computadores mudariam a natureza de ensinar e empresa da aprendizagem, as atividades diárias da sala de aula média não mudaram muito. A inteiro-classe, etapa do fechamento, apresentação do lecture domina ainda a paisagem pedagogical de a maioria de escolas.

Dado o potencial provado da aprendizagem computer-mediated e das falhas bem documentadas do lecture, que pode esclarecer a falta de progresso? Este papel examina os dois genres dominantes da instrução computer-mediated e sugere que as teorias da aprendizagem atrás de muito deste software conduzem aos projetos que não cabem com nem não mudam as estruturas básicas da participação da sala de aula. Nós discutimos que os objetivos do computador suportaram o conflito collaborative da aprendizagem (CSCL) fundamental com as teorias cognitive subjacentes de ambos estes estilos do projeto. Nós sugerimos então uma alternativa, o projeto centrado atividade (ACD). Acd, construindo em cima das introspecções da cognição distribuída (Hutchins, 1995; Ervilha, 1993) e teoria da atividade (Leont'ev; 1979; Wertsch 1979), vistas que aprendem como um processo complexo em que a cognição de um indivíduo é definida por sua relação ao ajuste do material e aos formulários da participação social incentivados por estes ajustes. Conformemente, o ACD emfatiza o projeto de ambientes computer-mediated para suportar e estruturar as interações e as interdependências de um sistema da atividade, including os interrelations entre estudantes, de seus instrutores, das tarefas que empreendem, e dos inscriptions se usam. Finalmente, nós crítica o ambiente do inquérito da probabilidade (TORTA), (Vahey, Enyedy & Gifford; 1999) do perspective da atividade centraram o projeto.

Projeto Centrado Domínio

O projeto o mais comum para a tecnologia educacional, especial ao último meio dos 1980's, foi " à tecnologia " de Bolton às práticas convencionais da sala de aula (Giffrod, 1996). A suposição teórica fundamental desta aproximação é o modelo da transmissão de transferência do conhecimento. O modelo da transmissão sugere que o conhecimento é um objeto identifiable que seja possuído por uma pessoa, destacada de todo o contexto social, que puder ser feito saber da mente do instrutor à mente dos estudantes. O foco do pedagogy deste perspective deve fazer a transmissão do conhecimento mais eficiente.

O convencional sala de aula-com seus lectures e textbooks da fechamento-etapa que apresentam o estudante com fatos, teorias e explanação-está uma extensão natural desta suposição teórica. A tecnologia educacional projetada deste perspective exemplified melhor pela instrução ajudada computador (CAI). Alvos do CAI para fazer transferência do conhecimento mais eficiente primeiramente requisitando a seqüência da informação e elaborando em sua apresentação. Por causa deste foco na estrutura do domínio disciplinary nós chamamos esta aproximação à instrução, o projeto centrado domínio (DCD). Poderia também ter sido chamado projeto de Professor Centrar-se, porque em a maioria de casos se supõe que o professor (se ser humano ou um surrogate do computador) é o único possessor do conhecimento disciplinary que deve ser transferido aos estudantes. Consistente com este perspective, quase todas as atividades da sala de aula revolvem em torno do professor ou do computador como o fornecedor e o avaliador da informação.

Figura 1 mostra o fluxo da informação em uma sala de aula centrada domínio. O professor é colocado no centro de figura 1 para demonstrar seu papel central neste tipo de sala de aula. O professor frequentemente prende o assoalho para até 80% do tempo da classe, deixando pouco tempo para um estudante a iniciado e termina uma troca instrutiva em dois sentidos. A tecnologia pode ser aparafusada sobre a cada um do aspecto de ensinar e sistema da aprendizagem de figura 1 (por exemplo realçando o professor lectures com as ferramentas da apresentação dos multimedia). Entretanto, fazer assim não altera o caráter básico deste sistema instrutivo.

O projeto centrado domínio e o modelo da transmissão da instrução foram criticados rotineiramente sobre os anos para o imitation e o memorization recompensar ao limitar detet-sense-making e diálogo significativo. Mesmo a interpretação a mais generosa do ¾ de DCD de que DCD envolve pensar crìticamente sobre understandings convencionais de um domínio, e as ferramentas e as respresentações de projeto que ajudam fazer o domínio acessível em uma maneira completamente diferente do que ela eram antes que o ¾ esteja ainda aberto ao criticism que ignora o contexto social da aprendizagem e suponha que de aprendizagem é primeiramente uma atividade individual (Lave, 1996). Esta suposição epistemological é o antithesis à aprendizagem collaborative. Bolting- na tecnologia a um sistema pedagogical danificado obviamente não se dirige a estes criticisms sérios. Em conseqüência, a aproximação de DCD à tecnologia instrutiva forneceu somente ganhos modestos da aprendizagem. Mais importante, baseado nos resultados último dos ambientes centrados da década domínio pareça destined ter somente o impacto marginal nas atividades diárias da sala de aula.

Projeto Centrado Learner

Porque a ciência cognitive e as ciências da aprendizagem se amadureceram, entretanto, tem assim a aproximação ao projeto de ambientes educacionais computer-mediated. O projeto centrado learner (LCD), emergindo do contexto do projeto centrado usuário da comunidade da interação de Human-Computer, propõe projetar a tecnologia da aprendizagem focalizando dentro nas potencialidades e nas necessidades cognitive do learner. Usando este modelo, o LCD produziu um número de ferramentas computer-mediated inovativas, visualizations e os micro-mundos visados ajudando a estudantes aprendem um domínio do específico (por exemplo Anderson, 1993; Branco, 1993).

A maioria destes ambientes da aprendizagem são fundados no modelo processar de informação da cognição que supõe que a inteligência humana é o resultado de nossa abilidade de operar sobre e transformar as respresentações mentais de um sistema físico do símbolo. A instrução portrayed pela maior parte nos termos de processar de pedaços grandes da informação codificada, unambiguous nas mentes dos learners. No campo da instrução, o modelo processar de informação conduziu a muitos estudos que focalizam nas potencialidades cognitive, misconceptions, modelos mentais e necessidades da aprendizagem de estudantes individuais. Isto é aceitado geralmente como uma melhoria sobre o projeto centrado domínio porque inclui o estudante como um participant ativo em his ou a sua própria aprendizagem. Por causa deste foco em indivíduos isolados e decontextualized mentes dos learners que nós chamamos esta aproximação ao projeto centrado do projeto learner instrutivo.

O modelo centrado learner do projeto é descrito em figura 2. Learner- objetivos, misconceptions e cognitive capacidade- é colocada no centro do modelo. Observe que a maioria dos elementos do modelo está a mesma que em DCD. O que mudou é seu arranjo. O estudante está no centro porque os ambientes centrados learner são organizados deliberadamente para aumentar o controle do estudante sobre arranjar em seqüência e o ritmo dos materiais. Enquanto o estudante progride através dos materiais está apresentada com o feedback baseado em suas ações com o software. Finalmente, quando o professor for parte do sistema, o papel do professor é frequentemente mínimo. O papel novo do professor no LCD é descrito frequentemente como, " guia no lado. " Entretanto, em a maioria de casos os tipos de sustentação fornecidos para este papel novo estão; non-existent, unrealistic (por exemplo dando ao professor a abilidade técnica de monitorar simultaneamente 30 investigações dos estudantes), ou no contradiction direto ao espírito do LCD (por exemplo dando ao professor a abilidade de controlar a tela do learner). Em a maioria de casos, entretanto, no professor o papel o mais eficaz e o mais direto é como o facilitator de uma discussão da sala de aula após as investigações locais dos estudantes. Entretanto, estas discussões não são suportadas geralmente technologically, nem são jorram integrado com a atividade local. O resultado é uma experiência completamente fragmentada para os estudantes, onde as investigações centradas learner são desconectadas com as discussões centradas professor.

Observe também que o estudante está isolado no centro do diagrama. A colaboração e a interação com pares não são necessárias central, nem uniforme, deste perspective. Um dos criticisms principais dos ambientes centrados learner do projeto e do modelo da cognição próprios processar de informação, é que promovem uma vista da aprendizagem como altamente individualistic, autônoma, non-social uma atividade *** TRANSLATION ENDS HERE *** (Lave, 1996). The LCD perspective has led researchers to design hermetically sealed learning environments which severely limit the range of social interactions with both one’ s teacher and one ’ s peers. Developing learning environments based on the LCD model is shortsighted on several accounts. First, it often goes to far in reducing the role that the teacher plays, denying the student access the accumulated wisdom, experiences, and empathy of expert teachers. Second, like Domain Centered Design, it ignores the social context of learning and the important role of conversation and collaboration as part of the active learning process. Third, the environments are focused on individual student misconceptions, which need to be changed to the normative conceptual understanding. While both naive and normative concepts are important to consider in the design process, they are situated in domains of practice – the embodied and contextualized knowledge of how to participate in concrete socially defined activities. LCD ’ s failure to recognize the irreducible, reflexive relationship between conceptual understanding and the activities in which they arise has led to compartmentalized learning activities that do not attempt to build off one another or connect with students ’ lived experiences.

The Need for an Alternative Framework for CSCL

While the recent work of the CSCL community avoids the narrow focus on the individual, much of the CSCL research is still attempting to build on the ideas and assumptions of Learner Centered Design. Instead of individuals however, at the center of the model are collaborative groups of students. While this is certainly an improvement, it repeats the errors of the bolt-on model of instructional design. In effect, it bolts-on collaboration to an individualistic and narrowly conceived view of intelligence without altering the basic activity structures or assumptions of the Learner Centered model of design.

Activity Centered Design

We propose Activity Theory (AT) as a starting place for a new theoretical framework for CSCL. The basic concept behind a theory of socially-situated and artifact-mediated human activity can be traced back to Lev Vygotsky (1978) and his colleagues A. R. Luria (1976) and A. N. Leont ’ ev (1979). At the core of AT is the idea that internal activities emerge out of practical external activity and therefore the unit of analysis must include the individual and his/her culturally defined environment (Wertsch, 1979). AT has already made significant contributions to the field of Computer Supported Collaborative Work (e.g. Nardi, 1996; Bodker, 1997) but has yet to make a significant impact on the design of learning environments (for a significant exception see Cole, 1996).

In developing a theoretical framework for CSCL, we propose to build on three of the central tenets of Activity Theory: a) that activity is mediated by cultural artifacts; b) that activity must be analyzed at various levels; and c) that internal activity (i.e. thinking) first occurs in the social plane (i.e. contextualized activity). Each of these insights will be outlined and their educational implications discussed.

The first insight of activity theory is the observation that culturally defined tools mediate all activity. From this perspective, mediation does not make tasks easier but fundamentally changes the nature of the task and can even lead to the creations of new types of activity (Wertsch, 1979). On this point activity theory is closely aligned with distributed cognition (Hutchins, 1995; Pea, 1993; Norman, 1991). This simple observation, that activity is mediated, has enormous implications for instructional design because it redefines the nature of learning. Instead of viewing learning as the rational abstraction of mental representations from one ’ s experience, learning is re-conceptualized as learning to participate in a cultural practice. Learning to participate in a cultural practice means moving from partial participation in that practice – where one ’ s participation is heavily mediated by more capable others (Vygotsky, 1978) and the physical constraints of the physical world (Hutchins, 1995) – towards full participation in the practice.

The second insight of activity theory that has implications for the design of CSCL environments is that activity can be analyzed on least three levels. At the highest level, activities are distinguished by their organizing motives or the object towards which they are oriented. Moving down in grain size, activities can also be examined in terms of their actions and the short-term goals. Finally, at the most detailed level of analysis activity can seen as consisting of specific operations and the concrete conditions in which they are carried out. For a detailed examination of the unit of analysis for Activity Theory see Leont ’ ev (1979).

These three levels of analysis contribute a rich perspective on the interplay between cognition and its material and social context. One way to visualize this more inclusive perspective on the context of cognition is the mediated-action triangle shown in Figure 3 (adapted from Engestrom, 1987). The vertex of Figure 3 shows that the "object " or function of cognition is mediated by historically and culturally constituted tools. The bottom three nodes of Figure 3 demonstrate the social nature of human activity ¾ that individuals (subjects) are situated in communities which are mediated by rules of participation and by divisions of labor (Engestrom, 1987). One way the mediational triangle can be used to inform the design of learning environments is as a "checklist " of the connections and interdependencies that must be considered. Applying this checklist to Learner Centered Design, one quickly sees that LCD ’ s myopic focus on the tool ’ s relation to the learner ignores the interdependencies between activity and its context within a community (represented by the bottom 3 nodes of the triangle).

The third insight of Activity Theory is that cognition and the cultural tools that mediate it have their origins in social interaction. In particular, it stresses that the higher order psychological functions develop first interpsychologically, and then are translated into intrapsychological, mental functions (Vygotsky, 1978; Wertsch, 1979). This has two major implications for the design of learning environments. First, it recognizes the fundamental role of social interaction and conversation in learning. This makes it a particularly attractive theoretical framework for CSCL. Second, it implies that the way activity (and cognition) are organized can only be understood by examining the historical context from which the activity has arisen. This means, as designers, we must pay attention to the interaction between multiple trajectories: the sociogenesis of cultural practice; the ontogenesis of people within a practice; and the microgenesis of ways of participating within that cultural practice.

Based on the implications of Activity Theory, we propose to design and analyze CSCL environments based on the Activity Centered Design model depicted in Figure 4. Instead of placing either the teacher or the students at the center of the model, we propose that the focus should be to design activities that help learners develop the ability to carry out socially formulated, goal directed action through the use of mediating material and social structures. From this perspective both other social actors and cultural tools are seen as resources that the students coordinate during activity. The layering seen in Figure 4 is meant to demonstrate that each activity is situated on a learning trajectory, where each activity is designed to build off and relate to the other activities. An attractive aspect of the Activity Centered Model stems from what it is not. It is neither teacher-centered, like the instruction provided within conventionally organized classrooms, nor is it student-centered, like the instruction provided by intelligent tutoring systems. The instructional settings afforded by both of these models of computer-mediated instruction leave intact the questionable presumption that learning consists the transfer of intact chunks of knowledge from either the minds of teachers into the minds of their students, or from computer-mediated instructional materials into the minds of students. In the Activity Centered Model, as students move through the activities they progress from being partial participants, heavily dependent on the material mediation of tools, to full participants, able to more flexibly use the cultural tools of the normative practice.

The Probability Inquiry Environment from the Activity Centered Design Perspective

In this section we briefly critique the Probability Inquiry Environment from the perspective of Activity Centered Design, taking each of the tenets is turn. PIE is a computer-mediated inquiry environment proven to help middle school students learn elementary probability (Enyedy, Vahey & Gifford, 1997; Vahey, Enyedy & Gifford, under review ; Vahey Enyedy & Gifford, 1999). PIE was implemented as a three week curriculum, which included computer-simulation activities, hands-on activities and whole class discussions. Each computer activity was designed to focus on a particular aspect of probability and to promote specific interactions in the classroom culture (Enyedy, Vahey & Gifford, 1998). In PIE, students actively investigate probability by trying to figure out if particular games of chance are fair to all participants. The students ’ collaborative activity is structured around articulating their intuitions, systematically testing their ideas by gathering and analyzing empirical data, and communicating their revised understanding of the domain to their classmates. The computer-mediated activities are then followed by hands-on activities in which students flip coins, roll dice, etc. as they investigate aspects of probability without using the computer simulations. Throughout the curriculum the students also participate in whole-class discussions, in which each pair relates their findings from the activities.

It is important to note that PIE ’ s original conception and design was best characterized as somewhere between the Learner Centered Design and Activity Centered Design models. It was during its implementation and subsequent analysis that our own theoretical perspective evolved into the Activity Centered framework presented above. This evolution creates some apparent conflicts between PIE ’ s design and our theoretical framework. For example, Activity Theory approaches to education are often associated with apprenticeship, whereas PIE ’ s approach seems to assume that students learn probability by doing scientific investigations. However, as we attempt to show below, we believe the students were actually learning mathematical practices ¾ ways of perceiving and talking about probability that were accepted by the classroom community as successful methods for justifying claims (e.g. the claim that a given game was fair).

Mediating Probabilistic Reasoning

When we examined PIE from the perspective of how it mediated probabilistic reasoning, we found that by the end of the three weeks most students justified their claims about probability by calculating the probability of a compound event (Vahey, Enyedy & Gifford, under review ). The students ’ practice in most cases was structured around the probability tree as an ordered inscription of the fully enumerated sample space (i.e. all the possible outcomes). Elsewhere, we have outlined the trajectory of this particular cultural tool within the classroom (Enyedy et al., 1997) and the different social contexts in which the tool was used (Enyedy et al., 1998).

The Interdependencies of Mediation

However, there is more to an activity system and learning environment than the relationship between a tool and a method of reasoning. In our analysis of PIE, both the division of labor between students and the "rules " by which they interact effect the organization of activity and ultimately the students ’ learning outcomes. Both of these mediating factors will be examined in turn.

We use Figure 5 to graphically show the reduced set of interdependencies we examine in our analysis of how the different configurations of who did what during the PIE activities effected the students ’ learning. In the prediction phase of the students ’ investigations we saw at least two distinct divisions of labor. Although the students worked in pairs, PIE was designed with only one text box to record their predictions. Dissent from their shared answer, was intended to be expressed through the use of "agreement bars. " One way in which the students organized themselves to accomplish this task was to alternate who was responsible for that particular question. Alternatively, some students attempted to reach consensus on each and every question. These two ways of dividing the labor within the constraints of the mediating tool resulted in different patterns of interaction and different learning trajectories.

Under the alternating responsibility method, points of disagreement between student understanding often went undiscovered or ignored. Alternating responsibility compartmentalized their answers and eliminated the need for coherence across the questions. This presented a difficulty for a curriculum based on the ideal of students refining their ideas because of cognitive dissonance. It effectively eliminated the social accountability for their answers, and as a result students using this method did not often refine their ideas based on the input of others. On the other hand, trying to reach consensus on each question had its own strengths and weaknesses. While the process of collaboratively reaching consensus made differences between students visible to each other, it did not always lead to deep reflection about those ideas. It has been pointed out that high bandwidth systems, which immediately attempt to reach consensus, tend to settle on a solution nearest to the initial center of gravity regardless of what the evidence suggests as the "best " solution (Hutchins, 1995). In PIE this meant that the students who tried to reach consensus on their predictions, often agreed on the first explanation that seemed sensible to them without fully exploring it or its alternatives.

What is clear from this quick look at some of the ways that students answered the predictive questions of PIE is that the tool that mediated the articulation of their intuitions (i.e. a shared space for answers) was in turn mediated by the way the students divided their labor. While it is unrealistic to think that we can predict or completely determine how students will use a tool, this example shows that in designing CSCL systems it is important to consider how the larger context of the activity system will mediate the tools use and the students learning trajectory.

We also examined how the participation structures (i.e. rules) for student-to-student interaction mediated the way in which PIE was used and what the students learned from the activity. Figure 6 shows the set of interdependencies we examine in the analysis of two students as they answered a predictive question that asked about the probability distribution of two coin flips. This interaction is shown in Figure 7.

FiguFigure 7: Rosa and Maria setting the frame for their interaction.

The first turn of this interaction shows Rosa attempting to establish a shared understanding of their current task by reading the Predictive Question into the public interactional space. Having a shared understanding of the task has enormous implications for what actually gets done and what the students eventually understand. What is interesting about this interaction is that the students do not read the entire question (In Figure 7, compare Turn 1 to the text on the top left of the computer display). The part of the question that they do not read aloud, is exactly the parameter of the task they end up ignoring. Even though the teacher in Turn 8 reminds them that they need to consider the total number of points of their prediction, the students do not make any attempt to make their prediction add up to twenty. In fact, they do not seem to be attuned to quantity at all. Nowhere in this interaction to they mention the cardinal value of any outcome or class of outcomes. Rather, they use relative terms like "higher " and "highest " to talk about the ordinal relations of the classes of outcomes. For this interaction, then, their activity only partially corresponds to the intended activity, because they negotiated the task to include only the relative value of the histogram bars. Even so, in Turn 3 and 4 we see that the two girls collaborating to create a preliminary conjecture that is backed by a justification, that in turn incorporates one of the inscription systems of PIE. That is, even though their assertion is incorrect, its form reflects the desired participation structure of a well-formed argument.

The Sociogenetic, Ontogenetic and Microgenetic Context of PIE

Finally, examining how PIE is situated with respect to the possible socio- and ontogenetic trajectories reveals both some of the strengths and weakness of our design. At the sociogenetic level, we find that PIE takes a somewhat restricted view of the context of probability in relation to the larger domain of practice. In all of the PIE activities, the context for probabilistic reasoning was analyzing games of chance. This corresponds well to the historical roots of classical probability in which probabilistic situations, usually games, are analyzed in terms the number of favorable and non-favorable equiprobable outcomes. It does not, however, address the many real world and far less structured contexts where students might profit by leveraging probability, such as the assessment of risk, or understanding the reliability of a medical test. Our restriction of the activities to game playing is likely directly tied to the students ’ limited success at probabilistic reasoning in contexts outside of gaming (see Vahey et al, under review ). At the ontogenetic level, however, we believe our choice of games was justified. Games and fairness are authentic interests of students of this age. The gaming context leveraged this interest and helped motivate the students throughout the activities. Finally, we found that students ’ microgenetic trajectories through PIE were fundamentally conversations anchored by the available material resources. In some cases, the inscriptions of PIE anchored these conversations in ways that helped them realize the relevance of the normative resources of probability which they previously ignored (e.g. the sample space). In other cases, the inscriptions conflicted with the students ’ intuitive practices and led them to totally reorganize they way conceptualized the domain (Enyedy, in process ).

Conclusion

There is still an enormous amount of research needed to develop our understanding of how the material, social and mental worlds interpenetrate in mediated activity. Activity Theory begins to lay out some of the dimensions of this task, but it is not yet clear how to apply the insights of Activity Theory to the design (rather than merely critique) of Computer Supported Collaborative Learning Environments. Activity Centered Design is an attempt to move us toward a more appropriate theoretical framework for CSCL environments that will lead to a number of concrete design principles, but this promise is as of yet largely unrealized. What ACD has accomplished to date is to identify and provide a unifying theoretical perspective on some of the major areas where design principles for CSCL are needed. The areas addressed in this article included: how cultural tools mediate cognition, how activity systems (and thus cognition) are mediated by social interactions and different participation structures, and how activity systems are situated in larger communities and their practices.

Acknowledgements

This research was funded in part by a grant from the UC Urban Community-School Collaborative. Additionally, Noel Enyedy ’ s research was supported by a National Science Foundation Science and Design Traineeship and a Spencer Foundation Fellowship. We would also like to thank Phil Vahey and Jesse Ragent for their assistance in the design and implementation of PIE, and Cynthia Carter Ching for her helpful comments on an earlier draft of this paper.

References

Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of the mind . Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Berson, M. J. (1996). Effectiveness of computer technology in the social studies: A review of the literature. Journal of Research on Computing in Education , 28, 486-499.

Bodker, S. (1997). Computers in Mediated Human Activity. Mind, Culture and Activity, 4(3), 149-158.

Cole, M. (1996). Cultural Psychology . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Engestrom, Y. (1987). Learning by Expanding: An Activity-Theoretical Approach to Developmental Research . Helsinki: Orienta-Konsultit.

Enyedy, N. (in process). Material Structures, Communication and Intuitions: Resources for Learning in the Probability Inquiry Environment . Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, CA.

Enyedy, N., Vahey, P. and Gifford, B. (1997). Active and Supportive Computer-Mediated Resources for Student-to-Student Conversations. In Computer Support for Collaborative Learning ‘ 97 , R. Hall, N. Miyake, and N. Enyedy (Eds.). University of Toronto.

Enyedy, N., Vahey, P. and Gifford, B. (1998). "...It ’ s fair because they each have two. " The Development of a Mathematical Practice Across Two Social Contexts, In the Proceedings of the International Conference of the Learning Sciences 98 , AACE, Atlanta, GA.

Gifford, B. R. (1996). Bernie Gifford on the changing educational technical landscape. Educom Review , 31 (4), 14-19 .

Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Kulik, J. A. (1994). Meta-analytical studies of findings on computer-based instruction. In E. L. Baker & H. F. O ’ Neil, Jr. (Eds.), Technology assessment in education and training (pp. 9-33). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Lave, J. (1996). Teaching, as learning, in practice. Mind, Culture and Activity , 3(3), 149-164.

Leont ’ ev, A.N. (1979). The Problem of Activity in Psychology. In J. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of Activity in Soviet Pyschology . Armonk, New York: M.E. Sharpe, Inc.

Luria, A.R. (1975). The Historical development of Cognitive Processes. Moscow: Nauka Publishers.

Nardi, B. (1996). Context and Consciousness: AT and Human Computer Interaction . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Norman, D. A. (1991). Cognitive Artifacts. In J. M. Carroll (Ed.), Designing interaction: Psychology at the human-computer interface (pp. 17-38). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Pea, R. (1993). Practices of distributed intelligence and designs for education. In G. Salomon (Ed.) Distributed Cogntitions: Psychological and Educational considerations, (pp. 47-87). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Pea, R. (1994). Seeing What We Build Together: Distributed Multimedia Learning Environments for Transformative Communications. The Journal of the Learning Sciences , 3(3), 219-225.

Vahey, P. Enyedy, N. and Gifford, B. (1999). The Probability Inquiry Environment: a collaborative, inquiry-based simulation environment. In the Proceedings of the Thirty Second Annual Hawaii International Conference on Systems Sciences.

Vahey, P. Enyedy, N. and Gifford, B. ( under review ). The Probability Inquiry Environment: Learning Probability Using a Collaborative, Inquiry-Based Simulation Environment. Submitted to The Journal of Interactive Learning Environments.

Weller, H. G. (1996). Assessing the impact of computer-based learning in science. Journal of Research on Computing in Education , 28, 461-485.

Wertsch, J. (1979). The concept of Activity in Soviet Psychology: An Introduction. In J. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of Activity in Soviet Psychology . Armonk, New York: M.E. Sharpe, Inc.

White, B. (1993). ThinkerTools: Causal Models, Conceptual Change, and Science Education. Cognition and Instruction 10 (1), 1-100.

Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in Society: The Development of Higher Order Psychological Processes. (Eds.) M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, S. Scribner and E. Souberman. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Authors ’ Addresses

Bernard R. Gifford ( bgifford@socrates.berkeley.edu )
University of California at Berkeley; Graduate School of Education, Education in Mathematics Science and Technology; 4533 Tolman Hall; Berkeley, CA 94720-1670
Noel Enyedy ( enyedy@socrates.berkeley.edu )
University of California at Berkeley; Graduate School of Education, Education in Mathematics Science and Technology; 4533 Tolman Hall; Berkeley, CA 94720-1670

Powered by CILTKN